Cognitive and biomechanical interactions in language acquisition: A comparative case study of English and Japanese teaching for nonnative speakers
Abstract
This study investigates the interplay between cognitive processes and biomechanical methods in language education, focusing on English and Japanese instruction for Chinese university students. A mixed-methods approach, combining quantitative experiments and qualitative feedback, was employed to evaluate the effectiveness of biomechanical teaching strategies compared to traditional methods. Over a 12-week intervention, students in the experimental group, who experienced biomechanical techniques involving gestures, physical activities, and multi-sensory inputs, consistently outperformed their counterparts in the control group. Notably, the experimental group achieved higher mean scores in vocabulary (English: 85 vs. 75; Japanese: 78 vs. 68), grammar (English: 82 vs. 73; Japanese: 85 vs. 70), and listening comprehension (English: 88 vs. 77; Japanese: 80 vs. 72), with statistically significant differences (p < 0.01 for most metrics). Additionally, effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were calculated to determine the practical significance of these findings. The effect sizes ranged from d = 1.60 to d = 2.08, indicating large and practically significant differences between the experimental and control groups. Qualitative data revealed enhanced engagement, memory retention, and motivation among students exposed to biomechanical methods. The cross-linguistic comparison highlighted that English learner benefited most from multi-sensory vocabulary acquisition techniques, while Japanese learners exhibited substantial improvements in grammar and listening comprehension through interactive activities. These findings not only demonstrate the adaptability of biomechanical approaches but also underline their potential for broader application across other languages and cultural settings. For instance, languages with tonal systems, such as Mandarin or Thai, could leverage gesture-based methods to reinforce pitch and tone distinctions, whereas languages with complex morphology, like Arabic or Finnish, might benefit from kinesthetic exercises targeting morphological structures. Furthermore, educational settings with resource constraints can integrate low-cost, physical movement-based interventions, making biomechanics a scalable solution. By tailoring these methods to align with specific linguistic features and cultural learning preferences, educators can enhance their global applicability, paving the way for interdisciplinary innovation in language teaching. The study provides strong empirical evidence supporting the integration of biomechanics into language education, emphasizing its potential to enhance learner performance and satisfaction in a globalized learning environment.
References
1. González-Carriedo R, Weiller K, Boyd RR. “¡A Saltar!” Using movement to enhance the development of bilingualism. Lang Teach Res Quart. 2020;7(1):1-14. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/26390043.2019.1703392.
2. Knudson D. Research on learning introductory biomechanics. Int J Biomech Educ. 2016;25(3):15-22.
3. Messum P, Young R. Teaching students to pronounce English: a motor skill approach in the classroom. RELC J. 2020;51(2):184-200. doi: 10.1177/0033688220964107. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0033688220964107.
4. Cheung W, Ostrosky M, Yang HW, Akamoglu Y, Favazza PC, Aronson-Ensign K. Merging motor and cognitive development: there’s so much to learn while being physically active! Early Child Educ J. 2019;47(5):529-38.
5. Beilock SL, Lyons IM, Mattarella-Micke A, Nusbaum H, Small S. Sports experience changes the neural processing of action language. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(36):13269-73. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803424105. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0803424105.
6. Alcock K, Krawczyk K. Individual differences in language development: relationship with motor skill at 21 months. Dev Sci. 2010;13(5):677-83. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00924.x. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00924.x.
7. Berninger V, Richards T, Nielsen K, Dunn M, Raskind M, Abbott R. Behavioral and brain evidence for language by ear, mouth, eye, and hand and motor skills in literacy learning. Learn Disabil Q. 2018;41(3):171-84. doi: 10.1080/21683603.2018.1458357. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21683603.2018.1458357.
8. Rodriguez AD, McCabe M, Nocera JR, Reilly J. Concurrent word generation and motor performance: further evidence for language-motor interaction. PLoS One. 2012;7(5):e37094. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037094. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0037094.
9. Bert G. Teaching and assessing manipulative motor skills in high school physical education. Phys Educ J. 2015;72(2):123-30. doi: 10.1080/08924562.2015.1002352. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08924562.2015.1002352.
10. Butler LK, Tager-Flusberg H. Fine motor skill and expressive language in minimally verbal and verbal school-aged autistic children. Autism Res. 2022;15(1):122-30. doi: 10.1002/aur.2883. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1002/aur.2883.
11. Zendel B. TIhe importance of the motor system in the development of music-based forms of auditory rehabilitation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2022:6:nyas.14810. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14810.Available from: htlps://dx.doi.org/10.1111/nyas.148101
12. Qi H, Zhang Q. Effective strategies for language instruction in physical education from the perspective of tacit knowledge. J Soc Sci. 2020;8(3):227-35. doi: 10.4236/jss.2020.83024. Available from: http://www.scirp.org/journal/PaperDownload.aspx?paperID=99000.
13. Vandergrift L. Facilitating second language listening comprehension: acquiring successful strategies. English Lang Teach. 1999;53(3):168-76. doi: 10.1093/ELT/53.3.168. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1093/ELT/53.3.168.
14. Logie R, Belletier C, Doherty J. Integrating ‘Iheories of Working Memory. In: ‘Iheories of Working Memory. Oxford UniversityPress; 2020. doi: 10.1093/oso/9780198842286.003.0014Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198842286.003.00141.
15. Hayashi K, Takahashi N. The relationship between phonological short-term memory and vocabulary acquisition in Japanese young children. Open J Mod Linguist. 2020;10(2):121-36. doi: 10.4236/ojml.2020.102009. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ojml.2020.102009.
16. Burca M, Beaucousin V, Chaussé P, Ferrand I., Parris B, Augustinova M. Is ‘Ihere Semantic Conflict in the Stroop Task? Journal of Psychology [Internet]. 2021 Dec 15; Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a0005301.
17. Barsalou LW. Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Top Cogn Sci. 2010;2(4):716-24. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x.
18. Kosaka T. The multiword processing by low-proficiency Japanese English learners: Meaningfulness and constructions. Int J Appl Linguist. 2023;33(4):547-60. doi: 10.1111/ijal.12528. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ijal.12528.
19. DiDonato Brumbach AC, Goffman L. Interaction of language processing and motor skill in children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2014;57(1):104-16. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0215). Available from: https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0215).
20. MacDonald M, Lord C, Ulrich D. Motor skills and calibrated autism severity in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Adapt Phys Act Q. 2014;31(2):151-63. doi: 10.1123/apaq.2013-0068. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1123/apaq.2013-0068
Copyright (c) 2024 Dazhi Wu, Juan Lei, Yanzhe Wang
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Copyright on all articles published in this journal is retained by the author(s), while the author(s) grant the publisher as the original publisher to publish the article.
Articles published in this journal are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International, which means they can be shared, adapted and distributed provided that the original published version is cited.